Chapter 11

Things to think about before reading this Chapter

  • How does the nature of friendship change across childhood and adolescence?
  • In what ways do the seven major theories of peer relationships reflect aspects of dynamic systems theories presented in Chapter 1?
  • What are the major methodological challenges faced when developmental scientists study peer relationships empirically?
  • How does cognitive development during childhood relate to the development of peer relations during childhood?
  • During childhood and adolescence, how are girls’ friendships different from boys’ friendships? How are they similar?
  • In what ways do individual traits or ability influence the quality of children’s peer relationships?
  • How are parental relationships related to children’s peer relationships?
  • How does culture influence the nature of children’s peer relationships?

Chapter Outline

PEER RELATIONSHIPS IN CHILDHOOD

Introduction

Theoretical Perspectives on Peer Relationships Research

The Developmental Course of Peer Interaction

Infancy and the Toddler Period

The Preschool Years

Social participation

Other early developmental differences

Development beyond the Preschool Period

Children and their Friends

Functions of Friendship

Children’s Conceptions of Friendship

The Prevalence and Stability of Friendship

Similarities between Friends

Behaviors between Friends

Gender-Related Issues

Children without Friends

Friendship and Adjustment

Friendship and shy/withdrawn children

Friendship and externalizing children

Summary

Children’s Groups

The Peer Group as a Social Context

Peer Group Functions

Cliques and Crowds

Methodological Issues in the Study of Peer Groups

Assessing peer groups

Examining the contextual effect of the peer group

Summary

Peer Acceptance, Rejection, and Perceived Popularity

Assessing the Quality of Children’s Peer Relationships

Assessments of Peer Acceptance

Assessment of Child Behaviors

Peer assessments of social behavior

Teacher assessments of social behavior

Behavioral observations of social behavior

Correlates and Determinants of Peer Acceptance

Behavioral Correlates of Peer Acceptance

Social Cognitive Correlates of Peer Acceptance

Cognitions and Feelings about the Self and Peer Acceptance

Outcomes of Peer Relationships Difficulties

Peer Rejection and Externalizing Problems

Peer Rejection and Internalizing Problems

Summary

Origins of Children’s Peer Relationships and Social Skills

Temperament and Peer Relationships

Parenting and Peer Relationships

Parenting Behaviors and Peer Relationships

Culture and Peer Relationships

Conclusions

Suggested Readings

Brownell, C. A., Ramani, G. B., & Zerwas, S. (2006). Becoming a social partner with peers: Cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. Child Development, 77, 803–821.

Chen, X., Chang, L., Liu, H., & He, Y. (2008). Effects of the peer group on the development of social functioning and academic achievement: A longitudinal study in Chinese children. Child Development, 79, 235–251.

Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association between aggression and social status. Child Development, 75, 147–163.

Coie, J. D., & Kupersmidt, J. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerging social status in boys’ groups. Child Development, 54, 1400–1416.

Furman, W., Simon, V., Shaffer, L., & Bouchey, H. A. (2002). Adolescents’ working models and styles for relationships with parents, friends, and romantic partners. Child Development, 73, 241–255.

Haselager, G. J. T., Hartup, W. M., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (1998). Similarities between friends and nonfriends in middle childhood. Child Development, 69(4), 1198–1208.

Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships and culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowksi, W. M. (1999). The power of friendship protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 94–101.

Krasnor, L., & Rubin, K. H. (1983). Preschool social problem solving: Attempts and outcomes in naturalistic interaction. Child Development, 54, 1545–1558.

Ladd, G. W., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2003). The role of chronic peer difficulties in the development of children’s psychological adjustment problems. Child Development, 74, 1344–1367.

Laursen, B., Bukowski, W., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. (2007). Friendship moderates prospective associations between social isolation and adjustment problems in young children. Child Development, 78, 1395–1404.

London, B., Downey, G., Bonica, C., & Paltin, I. (2007). Social causes and consequences of rejection sensitivity. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 481–506.

Parker, J. G. & Seal, J. (1996). Forming, losing, renewing, and replacing friendships: Applying temporal parameters to the assessment of children’s friendship experiences. Child Development, 67, 2248–2268.

Rose, A. J., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. M. (2007). Prospective associations of co-rumination with friendship and emotional adjustment: Considering the socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 1019–1031.

Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., & Bowker, J. C. (2009). Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 141–171.

Rubin, K. H., Watson, K., & Jambor, T. (1978). Free play behaviors in preschool and kindergarten children. Child Development, 49, 534–536.

Salmivalli, C. & Isaacs, J. (2005). Prospective relations among victimization, rejection, friendlessness, and children’s self- and peer-perceptions. Child Development, 76(6), 1161–1171.

Simpkins, S., & Parke, R. (2002). Do friends and nonfriends behave differently? A social relations analysis of children’s behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 263–283.

Strayer, F. F. & Santos, A. J. (1996). Affiliative structures in preschool peer groups. Social Development, 5, 117–130.

Wentzel, K. R., & Asher, S. R. (1995). The academic lives of neglected, rejected, popular, and controversial children. Child Development, 66, 754–763.

Glossary

Aggression: High levels of relational aggression (e.g., threatening friendship withdrawal) within the friendship, and high levels of exclusivity, jealously, and intimacy, characterize the friendships of relationally aggressive children. In contrast, overtly aggressive children direct their overt aggression outside their friendship dyads, and report low levels of intimacy (Grotpeter & Crick, 1996).

Conflict: Interpersonal interactions characterized by quarreling, active hostility (assaults and threats) and reactive hostility (refusals and resistance) among individuals.

Co-rumination: Intimate self-disclosure characterized by dwelling on emotionally charged and intimate everyday occurrences and feelings in negative ways.

Cultural influence: The impact of cultural norms and values in defining the “meanings” of social behaviors that affect group and individual beliefs, attitudes, and value systems.

Emotion regulation: The management of emotions to accomplish one’s goals.

Externalizing problems: Behavioral difficulties in adolescence such as delinquency, conduct disorder, attentional difficulties, and substance abuse associated with peer rejection in childhood.

Friendships: Close, mutual, and voluntary dyadic relationships characterized by reciprocity and a feeling of perceived equality between individuals that provide children with support, security, intimacy, affection, and both instrumental and informational assistance. Outcomes of friendships include consensual validation of interests, hopes, and fears, the creation of prototypes for later romantic, marital, and parental relationships, and growth in interpersonal sensitivity.

Internalizing problems: Behavioral and emotional problems across the lifespan such as low self-esteem, anxiety problems, loneliness, and depressive symptoms that are associated with peer rejection in childhood.

Peer acceptance: The experience of being liked and accepted by peers.

Peer rejection: The experience of being disliked and not accepted by peers.

Peers: Similar-age individuals with whom children socialize.

Perceived popularity: The degree to which children are regarded as are “popular,” cool, central, or highly visible by their peers.

Perceived social competence: An awareness of one’s own peer acceptance or social skillfulness; children with higher perceived social competence tend to be more popular with peers.

Rejection sensitivity: The tendency to defensively expect, readily perceive, and overreact to peer rejection.

Social information-processing: Rubin and Rose-Krasnor’s model of interpersonal problem-solving in which children sequentially select a social goal, examine the task environment, access and implement a chosen strategy, and then evaluate the outcome relative to the initial social goal.

Social participation: Parten’s description of six developmentally sequential categories of social interactions, including unoccupied behavior, solitary play, onlooker behavior, parallel play, associative play, and cooperative play.

Social skills: Behaviors and abilities that promote successful initiation and maintenance of social interactions.

Sociometric popularity: Method of categorizing children into status groups based on the number of positive (“like”) and negative (“dislike”) nominations received from peers.

Temperament: The biological basis of the affective arousal, expression, and regulatory components of personality that contribute to a wide range of developmental outcomes, including individual personality characteristics.