SOCIAL

PSYCHOLOGY

4th Edition

Eliot R. Smith, Diane M. Mackie, and Heather M. Claypool

Research Activities

Chapter 1

Research Activity

Perceptions in sports

Observe how people describe and explain the wins and losses of their favorite sports teams on Facebook or Twitter. What kinds of explanations are made for wins vs. losses? This activity becomes even more fun when you can compare the explanations made by fans on opposing teams after they have played against each other in a big game. Like the example from the book about the Princeton vs. Dartmouth game, do you notice that fans from the winning team are more apt to praise their teammate’s good sportsmanship, whereas fans from the losing team are more apt to blame bad calls by the refs?

Make similar observations after multiple sporting events. Do you see a pattern emerging? If yes, why do you think fans perceive things differently after wins vs. losses? Have your perceptions ever been biased in this way?

Research Activity

Constructing our own reality

Come up with an event you experienced together with a relative or friend. Write down every tiny detail you can remember of the event and ask your relative or friend to do the same. For instance, when you both went to a concert, try to describe independently in detail what the concert was like, what colors were used on stage, what the artist wore, and how the people that were standing in front of you looked.

Compare both descriptions and look for differences.

This exercise demonstrates that we construct our own reality (SP p. 15).

Research Activity

Me and mine

Imagine someone will be selling this pen. How much money should this person sell it for?

Now imagine this pen is yours and someone wants to buy it. How much money will you sell it for?

This activity demonstrates that people value “me and mine” (SP p. 17; also see the case study in Chapter 4).

Research Activity

Thinking Fast vs. Slow

Throughout this text you will see many examples of how and why people process superficially vs. in-depth. The website below provides a video interview with Nobel Prize-winning psychologist, Daniel Kahneman, who describes the characteristics of fast vs. slow thinkers. Watch the video and, as you do, figure out whether you are a fast thinker or a slow thinker. What are the consequences of being each type of thinker?

www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/video/slow-thinking-is-wise-thinking.html

Chapter 2

Research Activity

Underground Psychology

You might be surprised to learn that a lot of social psychology research has taken place on public transportation. Ever try to make predictions about who will sit next to whom or wonder why no one likes to make eye contact on the subway? Well, social psychologists have wondered the same things. The article in the link below describes a number of classic social psychological studies that have been conducted on the train, the subway, the metro––you name it!

www.slate.com/articles/life/transport/2009/11/underground_psychology.html

In the spirit of this these classic ‘underground psychology’ studies, you are encouraged to come up with a list of questions based on the above article and conduct your own observational field study. Once you’ve come up with your questions, it’s time to conduct your study. But don’t conduct your study just once. See what happens at different times of day and when there are a different number of passengers. What happens when smartphones and tablets are added into the public transportation mix? Share your results with your friends and cla ssmates and wow them with your empirical smarts.

This exercise provides a demonstration of how psychologists come up with research questions and then use observational studies to test them.

Reference

Vanderbilt, T. (2009, November 17). Underground psychology: Researchers have been spying on us on the subway. Here’s what they’ve learned. Slate. Retrieved from

www.slate.com/articles/life/transport/2009/11/underground_psychology.html

Research Activity

External validity and the Milgram experiment

If you’re having a hard time understanding external validity, consider completing this activity. In this activity you will learn about external validity by applying it to Milgram’s classic study on obedience. As an added bonus, you will receive practise on writing effective discussions.

Click on the link below to get started:

www.psychlotron.org.uk/resources/social/as_aqa_socinf_obexternalvalid.pdf

Reference

Sammons, A. (n.d.). Obedience research: External validity. Retrieved from:
www.psychlotron.org.uk/resources/social/as_aqa_socinf_obexternalvalid.pdf

Research Activity

The importance of replication

Imagine you have a theory that people in middle-class neighborhoods do not approve of rubbish being thrown out into the street. Ask five of your neighbors to answer a short questionnaire about their attitudes towards throwing rubbish onto the street. Ask three friends from class to do the same 1 week, 1 month, and 2 months later (consecutively) in their own neighborhoods. Compare your results to see if they all support your theory.

This exercise demonstrates the importance of the replication of research findings across various populations, times, and settings (SP pp. 44–45).

Research Activity

Ethical values

Imagine you are a community member of an Institutional Review Board. A researcher submits a research plan aimed at studying the effect of violent TV on aggression in young children. In the research, he plans to re-create a “normal” home environment, and have children aged 5–12 watch violent TV shows for 4 hours a day over a span of two weeks. The researcher plans to measure aggression by examining the attitudes of the children, as well as their physical behavior, after the study. He states that the children will only be slightly deceived: they will be told that they will be watching TV for 4 hours a day over two weeks, but the exact purpose of the study (effects on aggression) will not be made clear to them. He will, however, debrief them thoroughly after the study is completed.

What would you suggest to the researcher about this plan? Would the end justify the means? Would you accept the plan, or ask for some changes before it was accepted?

This exercise demonstrates the importance of considering ethical values in evaluating research that you read about, especially the importance of treating participants as fairly as possible (SP pp. 48–51).

Chapter 3

Research Activity

You’re so AVERAGE!: The effect of face morphing on attractiveness

What exactly makes faces attractive? Research by Judith Langlois and her colleagues (1990; 1994) [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00079.x] [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00503.x] contend that people all over the world find “average” faces to be more attractive. To test this prediction, the researchers took pictures of college undergraduates and then digitally morphed these faces into a collection of average faces. Participants were then asked to rate the original pictures as well as the averaged/composite pictures. The results showed that participants preferred the composite faces and found them to be more attractive than most of the individual pictures.

Don’t believe these results? Then put it to the test! Two researchers, Lisa DeBruine and Ben Jones, from the University of Glasgow in Scotland have created a website where you can take part in experiments on face perception and even play around with morphing faces!

To conduct your own version of the studies described above, click on the link below and hit the “Averager” button. Next, select eight faces male faces and eight female faces. Once you’ve made your selections, hover over each individual face and have some of your friends make ratings of each individual face on a 1 (very unattractive) to 5 (very attractive) scale. Then, without your friends’ knowledge, click on the same eight faces and click the “View Average” button under the large face on the left of the screen. Tell your friends to ignore the fuzzy images around the person in the picture’s head and just make ratings of the person’s face. Repeat these steps at least four or five times using different faces. Do your results show that your friends preferred the averaged faces over the original faces? If yes, then average does equal attractive!

http://faceresearch.org/

References

  • Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (1990). [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00079.x]. Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science, 1, 115–121.
  • Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., & Musselman, L. (1994) [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00503.x]. What is average and what is not average about attractive faces? Psychological Science, 5, 214–220.

Research Activity

Detection of deception

Ask people in your environment what they look for when trying to detect truth from deception.

This exercise demonstrates that most people look for evidence of deception in a liar’s face or words (SP p. 61).

Research Activity

I imagined you differently: Limiting the self-fulfilling prophecy

Self-fulfilling prophecies start with an expectation held by a perceiver. That expectation (whether it’s that someone is funny, moody, cruel, or kind), then sets in motion a sequence of events in which the expectation influences the perceiver’s own behavior, which influences the target person to behave in-line with the perceiver’s expectation, which then confirms the perceiver’s original expectancy. Given that perceivers are often completely unaware of the impact of their expectations, can anything be done to derail the self-fulfilling prophecy?

The answer is yes! The next time you are around a friend whom you think is introverted try to think of them as the most extroverted and outgoing person you’ve ever met. Let that expectation guide the topic of conversation and what you decide to do with that person. At the end of your interaction, reassess your original impression of your friend. Has it changed at all?

If you can’t find any introverted friends, this little experiment could also work with friends that have virtually any personality trait. Just expect the opposite and see what happens.

Research Activity

Integrating inconsistencies

Try to form an impression of the following people:

  • a person being social and lonely;
  • a person being stupid and brilliant;
  • a person being hostile and dependent;
  • a person being sentimental and a betrayer.

This exercise demonstrates that people can be very creative in integrating apparently inconsistent traits (SP p. 89).

Chapter 4

Research Activity

Name your choice

Make a list of 20 people you know (and have the required information about) and list their names. Next list their home town, their profession, and the street they live in, and then write down the name of their partner. Do you see a similarity in the names?

This exercise demonstrates the tendency people have to choose cities, jobs, partners, and streets with names that resemble their own names (SP p. 108).

Research Activity

I’ve got the funnies

Research on the facial feedback hypothesis by Strack, Martin, and Stepper (1988) showed that people who shape their faces into an expression of a smile actually feel happier than participants who purse their faces into a scowl. How on earth did they test this hypothesis? Well, it was pretty clever, and you can do it too!

As described in your text, Strack et al. (1988) had their participants make ratings of cartoons while holding a pen between their teeth or between their lips. Try it yourself and you will see that placing a pen in between your teeth makes you look like you’re smiling, whereas a pen between your lips makes you appear to be scowling. The researchers found that the participants who held the pen between their teeth rated the cartoons as funnier than the participants that held the pen between their lips.

First, find a set of ~10 cartoons to use as stimuli. You can collect them from a newspaper, or find them online (e.g., https://www.facebook.com/Bestfunnycartoons). Second, gather a group of friends and ask them, just for fun, to make ratings of cartoons while holding a pen in their mouth in one of the two ways mentioned above (you can assign one group of friends to be in the “teeth” condition and the other group to be in the “lips” condition––but don’t tell them about their facial expression or about your hypothesis). Warn your friends that they may look a little silly writing with a pen in their mouth, but you are their friend so you won’t judge. In terms of the ratings, ask your friends to rate each cartoon on a 1 (not at all funny) to 7 (very funny) scale. After the cartoons have been rated, find the average score for your friends in the “teeth” condition and your friends in the “lips” condition. Did you find support for the facial feedback hypothesis? Wouldn’t that be funny if you did?

Reference

  • Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.

Research Activity

Let it go

Research by Pennebaker (1997) shows that students who write down their thoughts and feelings about stressful events in a journal experience an increased physiological response in the short run, but better health in the long run.

The next time you’re going through something stressful, try writing down your thoughts and feelings and see if it helps you to feel better. In addition to feeling better, the processes of writing things down may also have given you greater clarity and closure regarding the issue.

For more information on the power of journaling, see the following sources:

http://psychcentral.com/lib/the-health-benefits-of-journaling/000721

Reference

  • Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic process. Psychological Science, 8, 162–166.

Research Activity

Attribution of success and failure

Make a list of success and failure experiences of the last couple of months. Then write down the cause of every single success and failure. After doing this, rate the causes as internal (i.e., you are the main cause) or external (i.e., the situation is the main cause). How many success events did you recall, and how many failures? Can you see differences in attribution of success and failure?

This exercise demonstrates the self-serving bias that most people have a better memory for success than for failure, and that they attribute success to themselves (internal causes) and failure to other reasons (external causes) (SP pp. 130–131).

Chapter 5

Research Activity

Are you sexist?

It turns out that sexism comes in a number of different varieties. If you’re interested in how you would score on a measure of sexism, take the following survey. This survey is designed for both males and females and, at the end, you can see how your results compare to the results of people all around the world. How do you score on measures of hostile vs. benevolent sexism? Do the scores you received correspond with how you see yourself?

www.understandingprejudice.org/asi/

Research Activity

Stereotypes

Think of a stereotype that comes directly to your mind. Is this stereotype positive or negative? Now think of another stereotype with the opposite valence. Investigate via the internet if you can find information that either confirms or disconfirms the stereotype.

This exercise demonstrates that stereotypes can be either negative or positive, and accurate or inaccurate (SP pp. 146–149).

Research Activity

Illusory correlation

Read the following:

  • Peter, a member of group A, gives his mother flowers.
  • Jack, a member of group B, leaves a nice note behind for his girlfriend when going to work.
  • Bill, a member of group A, nods in a friendly way to the woman behind the cash-desk.
  • Susie, a member of group B, helps an old lady cross the street.
  • Mary, a member of group A, gives money to charity.
  • Walter, a member of group A, whispered “bitch” when a woman accidentally bumped against him.
  • David, a member of group B, kicks his dog.
  • Robert, a member of group A, takes a candy bar without paying for it.
  • Jennifer, a member of group A, helps animals in need.

What is your impression of group A?

  1. = very positive
  2. = moderately positive
  3. = slightly positive
  4. = neutral
  5. = slightly negative
  6. = moderately negative
  7. = very negative

What is your impression of group B?

  1. = very positive
  2. = moderately positive
  3. = slightly positive
  4. = neutral
  5. = slightly negative
  6. = moderately negative
  7. = very negative

This activity demonstrates the illusory correlation: people tend to overestimate the incidence of negative behaviors among the smaller group (SP p. 151).

Research Activity

Perceptions of the homeless

Now, take about 10–15 minutes to complete the interactive demonstration found at the following website:

http://playspent.org/

Finally, answer the above four questions again. Have your responses/views changed at all? Many people endorse just world beliefs, such as those expressed in the questions above, because they like to think that things like homelessness could never happen to them. If we attribute homelessness to something about the person, something that they did wrong, then we feel that homelessness could never happen to us because we would never make the same decisions. Unfortunately, belief in a just world ignores the powerful role that one’s environment can play in a person’s situation. It is hoped that the interactive demonstration on homelessness sheds some light on what a struggle it can be to get by in society when you’ve lost everything.

If you’re interested in learning more about belief in a just world, please read the following article:

www.bedlammag.com/a-just-world-we-get-what-we-deserve/

Reference

  • Rubin, Z., & Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a just world? Journal of Social Issues, 31, 65–89.

Research Activity

What does the IAT really measure?

Research described in the text shows that people’s scores on the implicit association test (IAT) can be influenced by their environment. Let’s put that idea to the test!

First, take the Race IAT and take note of your score. The Race IAT may be found here: https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/

Wait for one day and then, before taking the IAT again, watch the following video:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=smEqnnklfYs

Now, take the IAT again and see whether your implicit attitudes have become a little more favourable toward Blacks. To learn more about the study that inspired this activity, read the article by Dasgupta and Greenwald (2001), and see how your results compare!

Reference

  • Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 800–814.

Chapter 6

Research Activity

Kids will be kids

As you are learning, in-group bias forms at a very early age as children learn from their parents, their friends, and their society around them. The next time you’re at a park with a playground, spend a little time observing the children. Do boys tend to play with boys and girls tend to play with girls? What about the race of a child’s playmates? As suggested in the article below, do children of the same race spend more time playing with each other? Finally, does age make any difference in who children tend to play with?  Put these questions to the test and see how early the tendency to favor one’s own group starts.

www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/06/110621074310.htm

Research Activity

Linguistic bias

Imagine your best friend is walking towards the supermarket. An old lady passes by with her groceries, but her bag breaks. All her groceries drop onto the street. Luckily your friend is not in a hurry, and bends down to pick up the old lady’s groceries, putting them in her other bag.

Describe this action of your friend in one sentence.

This exercise demonstrates the linguistic bias.

Research Activity

Out-group homogeneity effect

Describe which characteristics two of your best friends have in common, and which they do not have in common. Do the same for two people who are each other’s best friends, but who you do not perceive as belonging to your group.

This exercise demonstrates the out-group homogeneity effect (SP p. 201).

Research Activity

How does it feel to be stigmatized?

Do you feel bad for those who have experienced the negativity that goes with being a member of a stigmatized group? If you have never experienced anything like that, and you want to take a turn in someone else’s proverbial shoes, consider the following activity.

Buy a large adhesive bandage and place it on your neck or your face so that it is readily visible (a medical eye patch over one eye will also work). Next, go out in public for at least an hour and take note of your personal reactions and the reactions of others. Do you feel self-conscious? Do you feel like everyone is looking at you? If you speak with anyone, do you feel like they’re trying not to stare? Does anyone mention the bandage or do they all try to ignore it?

Consider completing this activity just to get the smallest glimpse of what stigmatized individuals experience every day of their lives.

Chapter 7

Research Activity

Subtle mimicry

Explore what happens when you subtly mimic people’s behavior. According to the text, individuals like those who subtly copy their nonverbals. Specifically, unobtrusively mimicking things like people’s facial expressions, gestures, posture, etc. has been shown to increase liking among strangers. So, let’s put it to the test!

There are a number of different ways that you can test this idea. First, you can subtly mimic the same person’s behaviors over the course of a few days and see how your interactions go. Then, you can stop mimicking their behaviors during the next few days and see if the interactions are less smooth.

Second (for an even more controlled activity), introduce some of your friends who have never met. Beforehand, tell one of your friends (who is a stranger to the others) to subtly mimic the behaviors of some of your friends, but not others. Is there any difference in liking for your friend? Do the individuals who were mimicked like your friend more than the individuals who were not mimicked? (After the fact, it might be nice to let everyone in on your little experiment.)

Can you think of another related experiment? Go for it!

Research Activity

Systematic processing

Find six people who share the same opinion about a certain attitude object. Then ask three of these people to mention three good arguments to support their opinions. Ask the other three people to come up with 10 good arguments to support their opinions. After everyone has finished this task, ask how certain everyone is about their opinion.

This exercise demonstrates that when you can easily come up with (three) arguments, you are (or stay) sure about your opinion, but when you have struggled to come up with (10) arguments, you are not so sure about it any more.

Research Activity

Analysis of Public Health Posters

Use what you’ve been learning in class to analyze public health posters.

First, go to the following website and find a few posters to analyze:

www.nlm.nih.gov/exhibition/visualculture/index.html

Second, answer the following questions about each poster:

  • What is the topic of the poster?
  • Are any heuristics being used?
  • Who is the intended audience of this poster?
  • What are the characteristics of the source?
  • Are they an expert? Are they likeable? Are they attractive?
  • What are the characteristics of the message?
  • Is it long or short?
  • Is the perceiver required to take the central route, or the peripheral route?
    • Is a fear appeal being used?
    • If yes, do you think the fear appeal will be successful?
  • Other noteworthy aspects of the poster?
  • Compare and contrast two posters. Are similar/different tactics being used? If yes, why might that be?

Don’t stop now! Now that you’ve analyzed these posters, move on to other persuasive appeals such as magazine advertisements, television commercials, etc. Do you notice any difference in the tactics used?

Research Activity

Persuasive appeals

Organize a discussion with six people about a certain subject. Before the discussion, measure these people’s attitudes toward the subject, and also ask how certain they are about their attitude. After the discussion, measure their attitudes again. Is there a difference in their attitude (certainty)?

This exercise demonstrates that people’s attitudes can become stronger when they have to resist an attempt at persuasion.

Chapter 8

Research Activity

Feeling the push and pull of attitudes

Don’t believe that your actions can influence your attitudes? Well, try it out for yourself! Gather together some friends and some candy and snacks of different varieties (ideally candies from the international food section that your friends have never seen before). Tell your friends that you are going to give them five snacks each and they will be asked to rate how much they like the packaging of each snack on a 1 (Very much dislike) to 7 (Very much like) scale. Each friend should receive exactly the same snacks (e.g., everyone gets one candy bar, one bag of chips, etc.), but you should ask your friends to do different things before rating the snacks’ packaging. For half of your friends, start with the snacks in a pile in the middle of the table and ask for them to bring the snacks toward them one-at-a-time, look it over, and then rate each snack in turn. For the other half of your friends, have them start with the snacks right in front of them, look over the packaging of each snack one-at-a-time, place the snack in a bowl on the other side of the table, and then rate the snack.

According to the research reviewed in the book, the act of moving the snacks toward them should lead your friends to like the snacks more than those snacks that they push away from themselves. Try it, and if it works, you can teach your friends that sometimes actions speak louder than words.

Research Activity

The cognitive dissonance effect

Imagine you want to raise the environmental awareness of your classmates. Most of the students probably say they believe in recycling, but you notice that their behaviors aren’t exactly in line with these attitudes: they often simply throw their paper in with the normal trash instead of walking over to the recycling bin. In order to address this issue, ask half of them to design a campaign about recycling for the other classes in the school. A couple of days later ask them about their own recycling behavior. You will probably see that the students you have asked to firmly support recycling will start behaving differently; they will recycle more. Why is this? What theory can be used to explain this change in behavior?

This exercise demonstrates the cognitive dissonance effect, and how behavior can lead to attitude change. The participants will experience dissonance because their own behavior (not recycling paper) is not in line with an attitude they strongly endorse (being environmentally friendly, and recycling) (SP pp. 281–284).

Research Activity

How do cults draw you in?

The study of cognitive dissonance began when Leon Festinger and some of his colleagues went undercover and joined a cult called the Seekers in the 1950s. The members of this cult believed that a UFO was going to pick them up to save them before the rest of the nonbelievers were wiped out in a great flood. The leader of the cult predicted that the UFO would descend on a particular date, so many of the believers quit their jobs and sold their belongings in preparation of the UFO’s arrival. Festinger and his colleagues joined with the knowledge that the end of the world was probably not on the horizon and that they could instead use the opportunity to study how the cult members would justify their beliefs and actions when neither the UFO nor the great flood appeared. Festinger’s classic examination of how dissonance processes shape later attitudes and behaviors became a landmark theory in social psychology. In addition, it turns out that the same dissonance processes that the Seekers used to justify their continued presence in the cult (after multiple end-of-the-world dates came and went), are also the same processes used to lure people into cults in the first place.

The History Channel aired a documentary on cults in 2006 that explored how cults form, and why even seemingly normal and psychologically healthy people join cults. Watch the documentary below (or even read Festinger et al.’s classic book) and see if you can point out the number of ways that dissonance is used to draw people in. Can you find examples of effort justification, insufficient justification, and how dissonance might be aroused and then reduced? If you were in a similar position, would you behave in the same way?

Watch the documentary here:

http://topdocumentaryfilms.com/cults-dangerous-devotion

Reference

  • Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails: A social and psychological study of a modern group that predicted the destruction of the world. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Research Activity

How attitudes can change behavior

Imagine you have decided you want to get into shape because you want to be healthier. You take a few minutes to think about how you can best go about this, and you plan out your intentions to use a weekly running schedule. Finally, you start following this schedule to reach the intended goal. As you go along, you monitor your weight loss and whether you are indeed physically healthier. What is this an example of?

This exercise demonstrates how attitudes can deliberately trigger behavior, through a process of forming specific intentions, activating behavioral information, planning, and finally carrying out the intended behavior. This also demonstrates the importance of motivation to engage in deliberate thinking before attitudes can change behavior (SP pp. 294–296).

Chapter 9

Research Activity

Expecting consensus

Ask 10 people to tell you which drink they think is better: Pepsi or Coca-Cola. In addition, ask them to estimate the percentage of other people who share that preference.

Check if people tend to overestimate the extent to which others agree with them.

Research Activity

Replicating Milgram’s other study

Most famous for his research on obedience, one of Milgram’s other studies is also a quite clever (although perhaps less shocking) demonstration of conformity. In 1969, Milgram, Bickman and Berkowitz observed what happened when one or more people stopped on the sidewalk and gazed up at the sky. The researchers simply counted the numbers of passersby who looked up as well and used that as a clever measure of conformity. Read more about the study at the link below, gather some friends, and see if you can replicate their results. This is guaranteed to be a foolproof––and fun––demonstration of conformity.

www.laymanpsychology.com/social-proofing/

Refererence

  • Milgram, S., Bickman, L., & Berkowitz, L. (1969). Note on the drawing power of crowds of different size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 79-82.

Mastery and connectedness

Think of a situation in which you accepted the group’s norm while privately disagreeing. Why did you conform? Think whether your reason fits the need for mastery, the need for connectedness, or both.

Research Activity

Minority influence in the courtroom on film

12 Angry Men, the 1957 movie starring Henry Fonda, portrays a classic example of jury deliberations. Watch the movie (try the link below, or rent the video) and see how dissenting jurors are treated. Does the majority or the minority win the day in this movie? What tactics do both groups use to bring other jurors to their side, and do these tactics accurately reflect the discussion of minority influence from the text? Finally, see if you can guess the order in which the jurors will change sides.

So, gather around the movie screen, pop some popcorn, and have fun applying social psychological principles to movies!

Reference

Chapter 10

Research Activity

Free hugs: Violating a social norm

Different cultures have different norms regarding personal space. In certain cultures, like the United States, people’s personal bubbles are generally large and individuals may take offense if their personal space is encroached upon. So, what would happen if you tried to violate this norm of expansive personal space by offering people free hugs? Watch the following video clip and then consider getting together with some friends and recording what happens when you offer to give strangers free hugs. Do most people disregard their usual demand for personal space and hug you? If you have male vs. female huggers, does gender have any influence on rates of hugging? How do people respond if they do not want a free hug vs. when they do what a free hug? How are the huggers made to feel when other people hug or do not hug them?

Have fun and spread the hugs!

www.youtube.com/watch?v=vr3x_RRJdd4

Research Activity

Turning the tables: Using the norm of reciprocity with customer service professionals

The next time you are dealing with a customer service agent over the phone, consider following the advice of Noah Goldstein (see the link to his article below) and unleashing the power of the norm of reciprocity. Before getting to the primary reason for your call, compliment the customer service agent on their friendliness, understanding (or whatever seems appropriate) and tell them that you would like their name and the name of their supervisor so that you can write a letter to the company thanking them for their help, after you hang up. Then, ask them to assist you with the original reason for your call. Because you have just offered to do them a favor, the customer service agent should feel compelled to help you with your request. As mentioned in Goldstein’s (2008) article, as long as you follow through with your offer to write a letter or provide some other form of positive feedback on behalf of the customer service agent, then this is a completely ethical tactic to use.

See if you can use the norm of reciprocity for good in other situations as well.

Reference

Research Activity

The low-ball technique

(a) Ask five people separately to help you to find a good article about the Milgram study on the internet. Additionally, make a second request to these same five people, asking them whether this could be done within an hour.

(b) Now ask five different people to help you to find a good article about the Milgram study within one hour, all in one request.

Compare how many people complied with the second request of procedure (a) as against the one request of procedure (b).

This exercise demonstrates the low-ball technique (SP p. 369).

Chapter 11

Research Activity

Social facilitation

Using social facilitation theory, think of things that you would do better with people watching, and things that you would do worse with people watching. Write some advice to yourself, describing how you could prevent your performance being worse with people watching in the future.

This exercise demonstrates social facilitation and its causes (SP pp. 397–398).

Research Activity

Task interdependence

For your next two group assignments, try to find out what form of task interdependence is at play (additive, disjunctive, etc.). Think of the consequences each member’s input can have, and see how this turned out once you’ve completed the assignment.

This exercise demonstrates different forms of task interdependence, and their effects (SP pp. 411–412).

Chapter 12

Research Activity

Investigating personal ads

Imagine that you are looking for a partner. Write a contact advertisement in which you describe yourself and what it is you are looking for.

Now go to the internet and look at contact advertisements of other people. Select two advertisements of women and two of men. For each ad, write down the factors that are described regarding the future partner. Do the same for your own ad.

Do you see differences between the factors that are described by men and those described by women? Do these factors correspond to the factors that can be expected according to the evolutionary theory?

Research Activity

Where did you get your friends?

A good deal of research shows that physical proximity is a strong predictor of who we like. Specifically, we often end up liking those that we are physically close to. To put this idea to the test, think about the friends that you’ve made since being in college. How many of your friends were:

  • the people that you lived with (or close to) in the dorms?
  • the people you sat by in class?
  • the people you saw at practice or rehearsal?
  • the people you were in clubs or other activities with?
  • the people you worked with?

If you formed friendship with any of the people listed above, then you have experienced the power of proximity. Next, poll your friends and be amazed at the wide reach of this effect.

Research Activity

Playing hard to get

To what extent would the advice to “play hard to get” be good advice for a woman who wants to be liked by a man?

When answering this question, think of terms like reciprocity, intimacy, and self-disclosure, and think of the effects of interactions.

Chapter 13

Research Activity

The relative deprivation theory

Imagine getting a computer for your birthday from your parents. You are very happy with your computer, but then you hear from two friends that they each got a new car for their birthday from their parents! Will you feel less happy now about having got your computer, or not? Why?

This exercise demonstrates the relative deprivation theory (SP p. 500).

Research Activity

Israelis and Palestinians - Working together to end the conflict

Explore the Israeli-Palestine Center for Research and Information website provided below and learn about the way both the Israelis and Arabs are working together to decrease conflict (click on the “Projects” tab at the top of the page). Can you find evidence of:

  • working toward superordinate goals?
  • looking beyond group memberships?
  • working together to find solutions?
  • other strategies designed to decrease the conflict?

Even in the midst of war, turmoil, and strife there are amazing people and organizations dedicated to decreasing conflict among groups.

Learn more at the following website: www.ipcri.org/

Research Activity

How conflicts can be resolved

Try to remember conflict you experienced yourself recently. Write the situation down, and explain how you resolved the conflict. After reading this chapter, do you think you could have resolved your conflict better? How?

This exercise demonstrates how conflicts can be resolved (SP Pp. 516–517).

Chapter 14

Research Activity

Helping

Try to remember five situations in which you helped. How did you feel after your help?

This exercise demonstrates that people feel good about themselves after having helped someone (SP p. 529).

Research Activity

Imitation and helping

Set up a situation in which you interact with a person. Just talk with this person about a topic, but try not to imitate him or her. Then, drop some pencils and register the other person’s reaction. Does he or she help you? And how fast? If he or she helped you, how many pencils did he or she pick up?

Now repeat this interaction situation with another person, but this time try to imitate him or her. Again, drop some pencils and register the reaction of your interaction partner.

Answer the same questions as you did for the other person. Do you see any differences in the amount of help that was given by your interaction partners? Did imitating your interaction partner promote helping behavior?

This exercise demonstrates that mimicry leads to increased helping (SP p. 480).

Research Activity

An analysis of helping behavior

In the text you have learned about a number of factors that influence whether or not people behave in prosocial and even heroic ways. Click on the link below and read about four or five heroes at the Heroic Stories Website. What makes these people heroes? Did similar factors motivate these individuals to behave in prosocial ways? If you were in that situation, would you have behaved in a similar way? Why or why not?

http://heroicstories.org/

Research Activity

Do a random act of kindness for someone else

What happens when you do a random act of kindness for someone else? Use the website below as an inspiration for ways you can brighten other people’s day. After each random act of kindness, answer the following questions:

  • How did the recipient respond to your random act of kindness?
  • How did showing someone a random act of kindness make you feel?
  • After engaging in this behavior, do you feel inclined to continue acting prosocially?

Visit the Random Acts of Kindness website for a list of wonderful ideas:

www.randomactsofkindness.org/kindness-ideas